CoDAS
https://codas.org.br/article/doi/10.1590/2317-1782/20212020200
CoDAS
Artigo Original

Quais fatores interferem no desempenho de pré-escolares no subteste de linguagem da Bayley-III?

What factors interfere with the performance of preschool children in the language subtest of Bayley-III?

Antonio Marcos Oliveira de Lima; Ana Manhani Cáceres-Assenço

Downloads: 1
Views: 607

Resumo

Objetivo: verificar se o desempenho de pré-escolares nascidos prematuros e a termo no subteste de linguagem da Bayley-III difere e identificar se as variáveis idade gestacional, peso ao nascer, nível socioeconômico e escolaridade materna são determinantes no desfecho de desenvolvimento de linguagem. Método: estudo transversal descritivo caso controle em que 36 pré-escolares nascidos prematuros e 27 nascidos a termo foram avaliados em relação ao desenvolvimento de linguagem pelo subteste da Bayley III. Foram considerados pré-escolares entre 18 e 36 meses de idade cronológica; com ausência de síndromes ou alterações genéticas, sensoriais, neurológicas, auditivas ou visuais; e que não tinham realizado terapia fonoaudiológica previamente. Os testes de MannWhitney, Exato de Fisher e regressão logística binária foram utilizados para análise estatística. Resultados: o desempenho dos grupos não diferiu seja pela pontuação composta (p=0,701) ou pela classificação baseada no percentil (p=0,225). A idade gestacional, o peso ao nascimento e o nível socioeconômico não influenciaram no desfecho do desenvolvimento de linguagem. No entanto, a escolaridade materna foi significativa (p=0,014) no modelo de regressão logística binária, sugerindo que a mãe ter estudado até a educação básica aumenta a chance de ter um filho com desempenho abaixo do esperado no subteste de linguagem da Bayley III em 6,31 vezes. Conclusão: não houve diferença entre os grupos no subteste de linguagem da Bayley-III e apenas a escolaridade materna influenciou no desfecho do desenvolvimento de linguagem.

Palavras-chave

Pré-Escolares; Nascimento Prematuro; Testes de Linguagem; Desenvolvimento da Linguagem; Fatores de Risco

Abstract

Purpose: to verify if the performance of pre-school children born prematurely and at term in the Bayley-III language subtest differs and to identify whether variables gestational age, birth weight, socioeconomic level, and maternal education are determinant in the outcome of language development. Methods: Descriptive cross-sectional case-control study in which 36 pre-school children born prematurely and 27 born at term were evaluated concerning language development by the Bayley III subtest. Preschoolers between 18 and 36 months of chronological age were considered; with no syndromes or genetic, sensory, neurological, auditory, or visual impairments; and had not previously undergone speech-language therapy. Mann-Whitney, Fisher’s Exact, and binary logistic regression tests were used for statistical analysis. Results: the groups’ performance did not differ either by the composite score (p = .701) or by the classification based on the percentile (p = .225). Gestational age, birth weight, and socioeconomic status did not influence the outcome of language development. However, maternal education was significant (p = .014) in the binary logistic regression model, suggesting that the mother having studied until basic education increases the chance of having a child underperforming in the Bayley III language subtest 6.31 times. Conclusion: there was no difference between the groups in the Bayley-III language subtest and only maternal education influenced the outcome of language development.

Keywords

Preschool Child; Premature Birth; Language Tests; Language Development; Risk Factors

Referências

1) Frey HA, Klebanoff MA. The epidemiology, etiology, and costs of preterm birth. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2016;21(2):68-73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2015.12.011 PMid:26794420.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2015.12.011
2) Ribeiro C, Lamônica D. Communicative abilities in premature and extreme premature infants. Rev CEFAC. 2014;16(2):830-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620143813
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620143813
3) Lee HJ, Park H-K. Neurodevelopmental outcome of preterm infants at childhood: cognition and language. Hanyang Med Rev. 2016;36(1):55. http://dx.doi.org/10.7599/hmr.2016.36.1.55
» http://dx.doi.org/10.7599/hmr.2016.36.1.55
4) Stevic M, Simic D, Ristic N, Budic I, Marjanovic V, Jovanovski-Srceva M, et al. Evaluation of factors for poor outcome in preterm newborns with posthemorrhagic hydrocephalus associated with late-onset neonatal sepsis. Ther Clin Risk Manag. 2018;14:1965-73. http://dx.doi.org/10.2147/TCRM.S177535 PMid:30349275.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.2147/TCRM.S177535
5) Synnes A, Luu TM, Moddemann D, Church P, Lee D, Vincer M, et al. Determinants of developmental outcomes in a very preterm Canadian cohort. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2017;102(3):F235-34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2016-311228 PMid:27758929.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2016-311228
6) Hentges CR, Silveira RC, Procianoy RS, Carvalho CG, Filipouski GR, Fuentefria RN, et al. Association of late-onset neonatal sepsis with late neurodevelopment in the first two years of life of preterm infants with very low birth weight. J Pediatr. 2014;90(1):50-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jped.2013.10.002 PMid:24148798.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jped.2013.10.002
7) Fernandes LV, Goulart AL, Santos AMN, Barros MCM, Guerra CC, Kopelman BI. Neurodevelopmental assessment of very low birth weight preterm infants at corrected age of 18-24 months by Bayley III scales. J Pediatr. 2012;88(6):471-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.2223/JPED.2230 PMid:23172131.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.2223/JPED.2230
8) Schirmer CR, Portuguez MW, Nunes ML. Clinical assessment of language development in children at age 3 years that were born preterm. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2006;64(4):926-31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2006000600007 PMid:17220997.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2006000600007
9) Guarini A, Marini A, Savini S, Alessandroni R, Faldella G, Sansavini A. Linguistic features in children born very preterm at preschool age. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2016;58(9):949-56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.13118 PMid:27061384.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.13118
10) Taylor R, Pascoe L, Scratch S, Doyle LW, Anderson P, Roberts G. A simple screen performed at school entry can predict academic under-achievement at age seven in children born very preterm. J Paediatr Child Health. 2016;52(7):759-64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jpc.13186 PMid:27189705.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jpc.13186
11) Bayley N. Escalas de desenvolvimento do bebê e da criança pequena. 3. ed. São Paulo: Pearson Clinical Brasil, 2017.
12) Macedo I, Pereira-da-Silva L, Brito L, Cardoso M. Male sex is an independent risk factor for poor neurodevelopmental outcome at 20 months’ corrected age, in human milk-fed very preterm infants: a cohort study. Einstein. 2019;17(3):eAO4607.
13) Madaschi V, Mecca TP, Macedo EC, Paula CS. Bayley-III scales of infant and toddler development: Transcultural adaptation and psychometric properties. Paideia. 2016;26(64):189-97.
14) Góes FV, Méio MDBB, Mello RR, Morsch D. Evaluation of neurodevelopment of preterm infants using Bayley III scale. Rev Bras Saúde Mater Infant. 2015;15(1):47-55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1519-38292015000100004
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1519-38292015000100004
15) Nielsen-Saines K, Brasil P, Kerin T, Vasconcelos Z, Gabaglia CR, Damasceno L, et al. Delayed childhood neurodevelopment and neurosensory alterations in the second year of life in a prospective cohort of ZIKV-exposed children. Nat Med. 2019;25(8):1213-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41591-019-0496-1 PMid:31285631.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41591-019-0496-1
16) ABEP: Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa [Internet]. Critério Brasil 2015 e atualização da distribuição de classes para; 2016 [citado em 2020 Jun 20]. Disponível em: https://www.abep.org/criterio-brasil
» https://www.abep.org/criterio-brasil
17) Capobianco M, Cerniglia L. Early language development in preterm children without neurological damage: a longitudinal study. F1000Res. 2017;6:1-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.13314.1 PMid:29445449.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.13314.1
18) Pérez-Pereira M, Fernández P, Gómez-Taibo ML, Resches M. Language development of low risk preterm infants up to the age of 30months. Early Hum Dev. 2014;90(10):649-56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.08.004 PMid:25189697.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.08.004
19) Monteiro-Luperi TI, Befi-Lopes DM, Diniz EMA, Krebs VL, Carvalho WB. Linguistic performance in 2 years old preterm, considering chronological age and corrected age. CoDAS. 2016;28(2):118-22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162015075 PMid:27191874.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162015075
20) Caldas CSO, Takano OA, Mello PRB, Souza SC, Zavala AAZ. Language abilities performance of children born preterm and low birth weight and associated factors Desempenho nas habilidades da linguagem em crianças nascidas. Audiol Commun Res. 2014;19(2):158-66.
21) Pérez-Pereira M, Fernández P, Resches M, Gómez-Taibo ML. Determinants of early language and communication in preterm and full term infants: a comparative study. Enfance. 2013;2013(1):59-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.4074/S0013754513001079
» http://dx.doi.org/10.4074/S0013754513001079
22) van Noort-van der Spek IL, Franken MCJP, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Language functions in preterm-born children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2012;129(4):745-54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-1728 PMid:22430458.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-1728
23) Nguyen TNN, Spencer-Smith M, Zannino D, Burnett A, Scratch SE, Pascoe L, et al. Developmental trajectory of language from 2 to 13 years in children born very preterm. Pediatrics. 2018;141(5):e20172831. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2017-2831 PMid:29632253.
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2017-2831
24) Roscoe-Bessa C, Pessoa MN, Dias ICB. Algumas diferenças comunicativas entre o português e o inglês. Cad Trad. 2016;36(2):91-104. http://dx.doi.org/10.5007/2175-7968.2016v36n2p91
» http://dx.doi.org/10.5007/2175-7968.2016v36n2p91
25) Ribeiro CD, Pachelli MR, Amaral NC, Lamônica DA. Development skills of children born premature with low and very low birth weight. CoDAS. 2017;29(1):e20160058. PMid:28146204.
26) Soares ACC, Silva K, Zuanetti PA. Variáveis de risco para o desenvolvimento da linguagem associadas à prematuridade. Audiol Commun Res. 2017;22:e1745. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2016-1745
» http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2016-1745
27) Abreu N, Miranda M, Bueno O, Martin RA. Pobreza e a mente perspectiva da ciência cognitiva [Internet]. 2015 [citado em 2020 Jun 20]. Disponível em: https://langcog.uni.lu
» https://langcog.uni.lu
28) Tella P, Piccolo LR, Rangel ML, Rohde LA, Polanczyk GV, Miguel EC, et al. Socioeconomic diversities and infant development at 6 to 9 months in a poverty area of São Paulo, Brazil. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2018;40(3):232-40.


Submetido em:
20/06/2020

Aceito em:
31/03/2021

619564dba95395550e08e4d2 codas Articles

CoDAS

Share this page
Page Sections